The developmental disorders, in any of its facets and diagnoses, such as
autism spectrum disorders, Deficit attention disorder with or without
hyperactivity, pervasive developmental disorders, generalized language
disorders, or development disorders No defined, impact on learning and the way
in which children attend to stimuli from the environment.
Usually specialized care programs focus on achieving as much as possible
behaviors of self-care in the best of the cases and even sometimes parents
receive the comments there is a little to do for their children because they
are not able to learn.
Although the subject of learning has been widely studied from different
perspectives, and in recent years thanks to technological progress, it has been
part of several studies with artificial neural networks, which have a point of
gear with the evolution of complex systems. The development of these enabled to
understand how nature has provided systems with the ability to adapt to the environment
that is the line that allows the survival of the species, which can be key to
work with children with developmental disorders, because allows to understand
development as a flexible system.
This is nurtured by other studies such as the plasticity and
applications of these principles whereas since the beginnings of neurogenesis
that are in the early stages of understanding, to idea of the connectome and
proteome that allow to understand the modeling environment achieved on
cognitive systems.
So the work with children with developmental disorders opens a door to
explore possibilities for psych educational treatment, leaving behind the myth
that "some children will never be able to learn".
Breaking myths
The widespread belief that a complex nervous system is required to make
possible adaptation to the environment either a change that triggers a new
reaction in an organism has been left behind and has begun to recognize that
chemical networks can evolve in simple systems that allow analyzing the ability
to operate routine by a body. Examples of this are the studies on chemical
samples created in silico or maybe the study of prions, which have opened
the door to understanding the mechanisms of adaptation and learning at the
protein level.
They are a physiological lesson and this is simple: adaptation to the environment is not a
mechanism created from a flexible brain learning, but a created evolutionary
inheritance so that species develop and survive because that possibly this same
adaptive response was inherited giving rise to other processes more complex as
neural networks that are currently studied as a whole with the name of connectome.
The connectome is a map of the neural connections, and which seeks to
describe the brain structure, as well as the genome is more than just a
juxtaposition of genes, the set of neural connections is much more than the sum
of its individual components.
The connectome contains millions of times more connections than the
letters of the genome, but also every who is creating specific connections from
interactions with the environment, so Sebastian Seung explains that everyone is
our connectome, which is formed based on 4 principles: reweighting which
means changes in the strength of the synapses; reconnection , which is
the creation and elimination of synapses; rewired which is the creation
and elimination of neuronal branches and feedback which is the creation
and elimination of neurons.
To this you have join the thousands of years of evolution in which these
processes are developing, because as explains Dehaene (2011) the brain
represents the response of the slow evolution of species governed by the
principle of the same natural selection that has been perfected over the years
optimizing brain the way it handles the huge flow of sensory information
received to suit the reactions of the organism to a competitive and sometimes
hostile environment.
This adaptation to the environment is the key to the survival of the species;
however, there are still changes that gestate from the available elements.
In a world made for man, wirings and neural functions would surely be
regulated by processes in perfect order and functionality from desirable
patterns, but nature is still experimenting with that has resources.
In this sense the natural systems continue to experience and making
adaptations in search of improvements, one of the first attempts to explain
this on a large scale was the so-called Baldwin effect, also known as
the ontogenic evolution which is a theory of the probable evolutionary
process of learning, which was published by first time in 1896. The theory
proposes a mechanism for learning ability in general, selected descendants of a
group, can have greater capacity to learn new skills rather than simply
the abilities granted by the genetic code which is relatively rigid.
However, I
must say the ontogenetic evolution
theory has received diverse criticisms, in part because it is very difficult to
monitor environmental changes in the superior species. But I prefer to see the
biological entities from a different perspective, so under the assumption that
the brain can adapt and learn from past experience, as the specific evolution
not only inherited behavior but that adds goals inherited which are used
to guide the learning under the orders of a genetic code that has two
components in the species.
In this respect is that the evolution of neural networks contains
information not only in genetic terms, but also a collection of behaviors
developed by the ancestors can be understood as the culture.
It is then that culture has a major role since the adaptations in the
environment are not always determined by closed code and therefore cannot be
stronger than those set by the selection (including changes in the social
environment).
This idea has generated several lines of research, and one of them is
precisely the assisted environmental neuromodulation.
The process of Neuromodulation
The process of neuromodulation is not new, arises from the observation
that different kinds of neurotransmitters in the nervous system regulate
different groups of neurons. In contrast to direct synaptic transmission in
which a process is required pre synaptic and other synaptic, the
neuromodulatory transmitters secreted by a small group of neurons diffuse
through large areas of the nervous system. Some neuromodulators are dopamine,
serotonin, acetylcholine, histamine, and others.
Neuromodulators are segregated in a natural way as a response to
environmental contexts or it can be applied in specific way that is the line
that the neuromodulation has developed mostly.
However this article seeks to focus on the implementation of contextual
programs for the acquisition of simple learning from environmental interactions
for the treatment on the developmental disorders, unless they require implants
or clinical procedures.
How does it work?, breaking environmental habits, creating personal
habits
Unlike assisted therapies, for example behavioral or cognitive-behavioral,
neuromodulation process applies in the environments in which the child is integrated;
this is your home, school, or anywhere where you are visiting.
The first analysis of the context consists of the family habits, which
are often laden with frustration and disorder. The process begin redrawing
environments and creating systems of habits in which all members of the family
can feel good.
Once it is designed the set of habits explores what however is able
to do as opposed to traditional therapies that focus on the dis
ability, this model seeks to observe the cognitive framework that will allow
the creation of new tasks and processes.
Sensory processes
Parents or caregivers take care of a program designed exclusively for
each child that has as its goal the shaping of specific tasks required within the
environment, for example, it is common to find delays in the acquisition of
speech, but understanding of language in children, partly because parents,
noting the delay in the development, won't stimulate the children become translators
of children by what is taught to parents to encourage the child, starting with
simple words.
One of the first words that develops easily is water, provided to the
person in charge whenever the child is facing the stimulus say water, and
occurs during diverse occasions and contexts during the day, for example when
children are washing their hands, to
drinking it, at the time of daily hygiene, at the beginning only says the word
slowly without having to finish the child repeat it nor whether this puts all
his attention to the stimulus, the goal is that is heard in the context
of the word.
Soon, children begin to either try to use the word or you are able to
use it properly. If the goal is achieved, then begins with adjectives like
water cold, hot, delicious, fresh.
One of the mistakes is that I forced children to perform tasks that the
adult may be simple, but that for a nervous system that does not yet integrate
stimuli becomes extremely complex, therefore it modulates the task,
dividing it into subtasks on the assumption of sensory integration planned.
Returning the example of water, this concept is a succession of 3 sounds
arranged in a certain way, it is an object, form, texture and temperature, by
what is heard, looks, feels, so for the apprehension of the same, is first
exposed to the small to the sound, then to the view and then to the touch, so
provided the sensory system is able to recognize this differently and if
any area of the brain is affected, it is possible to acquire it by any other
sensory input.
So the division of tasks is important for understanding and
consolidation of stimuli, which are located in the immediate environment and
allow actor learned, because it is common that therapies clinic learn concepts
that are not able to reproduce in other environments.
In this case it is possible to use different objects that approach
clearly and relaxed the child into the world that surrounds him.
What is different about this model?
This way to work with children begin with the assumption that all brains, no matter the
degree of physio-anatomic damage are able to learn under the right conditions,
and controlled, leaving you free to explore modeling.
There is no right or wrong answers, if the child is not able to
accomplish the task, it doesn't matter, because it will longer be able to do it
tomorrow, since there are ideal, every child is unique.
The child adapts to the environment and not the environment to the
child. When it is taught to adapt to the environment, there is less tension,
frustration and calm allows much more than the traditional behavioral
therapies.
Attention is molded in minimum time, beginning with 5 seconds, 6
seconds, and 7 seconds and on, do not force the child to keep the attention for
long periods it is known that this does not always work and is frustrating.
This way attention is sustained and
effective without forcing the sensations.
The guidance of the therapist is important because along with weekly
reports are allowed to build the program to follow, because this is not a rigid
model, but a flexible framework that allows learning modeling and accompanied.
The goals are short, medium, and long
term. There are no magic formulas, only motivation to prevent cognitive
Kickback.
No scolding, no punishments, children and parents learn to appreciate the achievements by
small that are and is constructed with a solid base of learning, from habits
that give confidence to the child, as the center of this therapeutic modality
is the idea that if you are able to learn, from a natural endowment which looks
for the survival of the species.
References:
Ackley, D., and Littman, M. (1991) Interactions
between learning and evolution. In CG Langton, C Taylor, JD Farmer & S,
Rasmussen (1991) Artificial life II, SFI studies in the sciences of
complexity. Vol
X. United States. Addison –Wesley.
Baldwin, JA.
(1896) A New Factor in Evolution. American
Naturalist 30, 441-451, 536-553. Disponible en http://www.brocku.ca/MeadProject/Baldwin/Baldwin_1896_h.html
Barret, HC.
(2012) A hierarchical model of the evolution of brain specializations. Proceedings
of the National Academy of Science of the United States of America. 19
(Supl 1). 10733- 10740.
Biswal BB, Mennes M, Zuo XN, Gohel S, Kelly C, Smith,
SM, Beckmann, CF, Adelstein, JS, Buckner RL, Colcombe S, et al (2010) Toward discovery science of human brain
function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 107
(10) 4734-4740.
Conrad, M. (2004) Evolutionary learning circuits. Journal of
theoretical Biology. 46 (1) 167-188.
Dehaene, S.
(2004) Evolution of human cortical circuits for Reading and arithmetic: the neuronal
recycling hypothesis. In S. Dehaene, J. R. Duhamel, M. Hauser & G.
Rizzolatti (Eds.), From monkey brain to human brain (2004). Cambridge,
Massachusetts: MIT Press.
Dehaene, S.,
and Cohen, L. (2007) Cultural Recycling of cortical maps. Neuron. 56 (2) 384-398.
Dehaene, S.
(2009) Reading in the brain:The science
and evolution of a human invention. USA. Viking: Penguin Group.
Dehaene, S.,
Cohen, L., Sigman, M. y Vinckier, F. (2005) The neural code for written words:
a proposal. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 9,
335-341.
Dzib, A. (2010) Alteraciones del desarrollo por
dificultades perinatales y la confusión con los trastornos del espectro
autista. Revista de Neuropsicología. 5
(1) 4-9.
Dzib Goodin, A. (2012) El virus HHV-6 y sus efectos en
el neurodesarrollo: un estudio de caso. Revista
Mexicana de Neurociencia.13 (3) 150-153.
Ehn, M., &
Laland, K. (2012) Adaptative strategies for cumulative cultural learning. Journal of Theoretical Biology. 301.
103-111.
Fernando, C., and Szathmáry, E. (2010) Natural
selection in the brain. In B., Glatzeder, V. Goel, and A. Muller (Eds) Towards
a theory of thinking: building blocks for a conceptual framework. Springer.
Germany.
Fernando, C.,
Szathmáry, F., and Husbands, P. (2012) Selectionist and evolutionary approaches
to brain function. A critical appraisal. Frontiers in Computational
Neuroscience. 6 (Art. 24). Disponible
en: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3337445/pdf/fncom-06-00024.pdf.
Gauci, J., and Stanley,
K.O. (2010) Autonomous evolution of topographic regularities in artificial
neural networks. Neural Computation 22 (7)
1860-1898.
Haesler, S. (2007) Programmed for speech. Scientific American Mind.
18 (3) 66-71.
Hinton, GE.,
and Nowlan, SJ. (1987) How learning can guide evolution. Complex Systems.
1. 495-502.
Ionescu, T.
(2011) Exploring the nature of cognitive flexibility. New Ideas in
Psychology. 30 (2) 190-200.
Iriki, A., and
Taoka, M. (2012) Triadic (ecological, neural, cognitive) niche construction: a
scenario of human brain evolution extrapolating tool use and language from the
control of reaching actions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society
Biological Science. 367. 10-23.
Kong, Q.,
Goldfarb, L., Gabizon, R., Montagna, P., Lugaresi, E., Piccardo, P., Petersen,
RB., Parchi, P., Chen, SG., Capellari, S., Ghetti, B. (2004) Inherited Prion
Diseases. In Prion Biology and Diseases. Cold Springs Harbor Laboratory
Press.
Krushner, D.
(2011) The man who builds brains. The Brain, Discovery Magazine.
Disponible en red: http://discovermagazine.com/2009/dec/05-discover-interview-the-man-who-builds-brains
Leonard, L.
(2002) Children with specific language
impairment. Boston: MIT Press.
Li, J.,
Browning, S., Mahal, SP., Oelschlegel, AM., Weissmannm, C. (2010) Darwinian
evolution of prions in cell culture. Science. 327 (5967) 869-872.
Lieberman, P.
(2002) On the nature and evolution of the neural bases of human language, American Journal of Physiology and
Anthropology. 45. 36-62.
Liu, Q., Zhou,
H., Cui, J., Cao, Z., & Xu, Y. (2012) Reconsieraction of In-Silico SiRNA design
based on feature selection: A cross-plattform data integration perspective. Plos One.7 (5) e37879.doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0037879
Lock, A., and
Gers, M. (2012) The cultural evolution of written language and its effects: A
Darwinian process from prehistory to the modern day. In ELL, Grigorenko, E.
Mambrino, DD, Preis (Eds) (2012) Writing:
A mosaic of new perspectives. New York. Psychology Press: Taylor &
Francis Group.
Lotem, A.,
& Halpern, JY. (2012) Coevolution of learning and data-adquisition mechanisms:
a model for cognitive evolution. Philosophical
transactions the Royal Society: Biological Science. 367 (1063) 2686-2694.
Marin Padilla, M. (2001) Evolución de la estructura de
la neocorteza del mamífero: nueva teoría citoarquitectónica. Rev Neurol. 33
(9): 843-853.
McGilchrist, I.
(2011) The divided brain. RSA Animated.
Disponible en red: http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=dFs9WO2B8uI.
McGregor, S.
Vasas, V., Husbands, Evolution of associative learning in chemical networks. Plos
Computational Biology. Disponible en http://www.ploscompbiol.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pcbi.1002739
Mennes, M.,
Kelly, C., Colcombe, S., Castellanos, FX., y Milham, MP. (2012) The extrinsic
and intrinsic functional architectures of the human brain are not equivalent. Cerebral Cortex. Disponible en red: http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/content/early/2012/01/31/cercor.bhs010.full.pdf+html
Miikkulainen,
R., Feasly, E., Hohnson, L. Karpov, I., Rajagopalan, P., Rawal, A., and Tansey,
W. (2012) Multiagent learning through neuroevolution. Advances in
Computational Intelligence. 7311. 24-46.
Nolfi, S., and Parisi, D (1994). Good teaching inputs
do not correspond to desired responses in ecological neural networks. Neural
Processing Letters 1 no. 2 (11/94) pp. 1-4.
Nolfi, S., y
Parisi, D. (1996) Learning to adapt to changing enviroments in evolving neural
networks. Adaptative Behavior. 5 (1) 75-88.
Nolfi, S., Elman, J., and Parisi, D. (1994). Learning
and evolution in neural networks. Adaptive
Behavior 2 (1994): 5-28.
Olarreta, A. (2005) Orígenes del lenguaje y selección natural. Sirius. Madrid.
Seung, HS.
(2012) Connectome: How the Brain's Wiring Makes Us Who We Are. New York:
Houghton Mifflin Harcout.
Seung, S.
(2010) Sebastian Seung: I am connectome. TED Talks. Disponible en http://www.ted.com/talks/sebastian_seung.html
Tubino, M. (2004) Plasticidad y evolución: papel de la
interacción cerebro – entorno. Revista de estudios neurolingüsticos. 2 (1) 16-25.
Valerio, LG.,
and Choudhuri, S. (2012) Chemoinformatics and chemical genomics: potential
utility of in silico methods. Journal of
Applied Toxicology. 32 (11) 880-889.
TEDxCaltech
(2013) The remarkable Neuron: Erin Shuman at TEDxCaltech. Disponible en red: http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=yr6kh_QOk0s
This comment has been removed by a blog administrator.
ReplyDeleteThis comment has been removed by the author.
Delete