The term plasticity
was introduced in 1890 by the American psychologist William James, in which
described the natural modificability of human behavior. Although in the last
years of the 19th century, Santiago Ramón y Cajal proposed that these
behavioral modifications would surely have a anatomical substrate, attributable
to the brain, and those changes of variable duration in synaptic function which
arise with origin in external stimuli affecting learning, are modificable by
this plasticity. Thus Lugaro and Ramón y Cajal deduced it almost at the same,
with different variations of ideas, both explained that learning involves
functional plastic changes in the properties of the neurons or their
interconnections.
From this perspective, learning may be the result of a
morphological change among the interconnections of nerve cells, similar to the
phenomena that occur during the formation of synapses in the embryonic life,
however, after Cajal’s death, it was adopted a rigid way of looking at the adult
central nervous system and it was accepted the idea that once finished its
development, anatomy of the brain remained unchanged, except for the
degenerative processes (cited in Nieto, 2003).
Since then, the concept of synaptic plasticity has come developed mainly in studies related to
memory and learning.
However, even after years evidence about the capacity of our
brain to change its functions and to compensate some damage, the importance of
this role has come to be appreciated only recently, since studies brain in late
19th century and early 20th focused on the identification of areas of specific
performance which gave the idea of a brain that governs its functions in
specific areas and thus the widespread idea in the psychology of learning must
be obtained in specific periods of time.
Some persons believe, this was thanks to Paul Broca, who
in the mid-19th century, identified a certain area in the left frontal
lobe-related to language, which was the starting point for the neurosciences
focused strictly on localizations of processes. Since then, many others
continued describing specific brain areas with specialized functions, such as
Broadmann; who described them but with more and more improved of morphological
techniques, architectural and neurochemical studies, researchers discovered
more details of the brain structure and functional connections.
The enormous complexity of the brain may have contributed
to the conceptual rigidity that was developed
in those years, since its organization was known within a whole, the anatomists
had to sectorize such knowledge. This motivated to Broadmann to divide the
cortex into 52 regions, and descriptions made by constituents showed them
separate, and this gave another reason to believe in concept of a rigid,
strictly divided brain. This, coupled with studies of connectivity and the
absence of concrete evidence of regeneration in the brain (in contrast to
organs like the liver which has the capability of mitotic cell duplication),
led to believe that it was a body divided in compartments, not malleable (not
plastic) and with little ability to recovery after injury, so few Anatomists,
physiologists or clinical projected a concept of dynamic adaptability of the
brain (Aguilar, 2003b; Poch, 2001, Aguilar, 2005).
Currently is kind of a
simple accept that an adult 30 years old knows much more than
a child of 10 years old, and at 70 years old someone knows more than during 20’s,
because the cognitive development process goes hand in hand with developing
brain in particular the development of adapted neural networks that allow
respond to the environment, and all this depends on the genetic information which is endowed to
each individual as well as from the mechanisms of adaptation of the environment,
so these days, such assertions become searchable thanks to the study of the
process called neuroplasticity or brain plasticity (Tubino, 2004 and
Ginarte, 2007).
However, it was not until some years later that this
concept of neuroplasticity
was defined by Gollini (1981) and Kaplan (1983) as a potential of nervous system to change (although it has observed
this same capacity in other systems such as the endocrine, respiratory and
skeletal muscle). This capacity can modify behavior and allow the adaptation to
the environment patterns of conduct, so this ability of central nervous system allows
it to never be finished and always change and adjusting as result of the
interaction of factors genetic and cultural, but also it’s know that this
ability decreases as neurons specialize (Bergado cited in Ginarte, 2007; Poch,
2001).
Defined more broadly, plasticity
is the functional adaptation of the central nervous system to minimize the
effects of the structural or physiological alterations, regardless of the original
cause. This is possible thanks to the faculty of the nervous system to
experience structural-functional changes detonated by influences endogenous
(internal) or exogenous (external), which can occur at any time of life. Some
researchers explain that this includes learning in its entirety; more
specifically, there is evidence of morphological changes such as neuronal
branching after learn a new skill.
While another group of experts, with a more intermediate
position, considered it as adaptive capacity of the central nervous system to
modify its own structural and functional organization, since plasticity
mechanisms may include neurochemical changes, at the parenquima, receivers or structures.
Likewise, functional plasticity is accompanied by a structural plasticity,
since there is also evidence of cooperation between brain areas (Aguilar,
2003b).
In the same way, it has been observed that there is
also a great ability to communicate between neuron-glia, which collaborates on
brain plasticity (either by creating new connections or removal and cleaning of
the existing) (Aguilar, 2003a, b).
In response, it should be recalled that major cellular
kinds of nervous tissue are the neurons and glial cells. Neurons, cells that
are highly specialized in quick, message reception and transmission have a
small body and multiple branches that cover an extensive area, allowing you to
optimize your intercom, making them malleable to the needs of the cerebral
environment (Nieto, 2003).
It is thus that the synaptic strength can be altered in
the different periods of development and range from milliseconds to months.
The cellular mechanisms of these changes are transitional
modifications of neurotransmission and in longer alterations, changes in gene
expression, so it can be said that there is a continuous renovation of the organization
and neuronal maturation (Aguilar, 2003a; Aguilar, 2003b; Castroviejo, 1996;
Poch, 2001).
References:
Aguilar, F. (2003 a) Plasticidad cerebral: parte 2. Rev Med IMSS. 41 (2) 133-142.
Aguilar, F. (2003 b) Plasticidad cerebral: parte 1. Rev Med IMSS. 41(1) 55-64.
Aguilar, F. (2005) Razones biológicas de la plasticidad cerebral y la
restauración neurológica. Revista Plasticidad y Restauración Neurológica. Vol. 4 Num.1. 5-6.
Castroviejo, P. (1996) Plasticidad cerebral. Revista de Neurología 24 (135) 1361-1366.
Ginarte, Y. (2007) La neuroplasticidad como base biológica de la
rehabilitación cognitiva. Geroinfo.
Vol. 2. No. 1. 31-38
Gollin.
E. S. (1981) Developmental and
plasticity: behavioral and biological aspects of variation in developmental.
New York. Academic Press.
Kaplan,
B. A. (1983) Developmental psychology:
historical and philosophical learning. New
Jersey. Elrbaum Hillsdale.
Nieto, M. (2003) Plasticidad neural. Mente
y cerebro. O3. 72-80.
Poch, M.L. (2001) Neurobiología del
desarrollo temprano. Contextos educativos. 4. 79-94.
Tubino, M. (2004) Plasticidad y evolución: papel de la interacción
cerebro – entorno. Revista de estudios
neurolingüsticos. Vol. 2, número 1. 21-39
No comments:
Post a Comment